2002 Prehistoric Painting, Sound and Rocks, from Studien zur Musikarchaologie III, The Archaeology of Sound: Origin and Organization, Deutsches Archaologisches Institut, Istanbul, Verlag Marie Leidorf GmbH, Rahden, Westf.
Saturday, July 13, 2019
ROCK ART AND ACOUSTICS: INTENTIONAL, ACCIDENTAL, OR IRRELEVANT? - PART 1
Two Mammoths, Rouffignac,
France. Marking a spot of increased
resonance according to
Reznikoff (2002:48).
Photo: bradshawfoundation.com.
There is a
great deal of interest currently in archaeoacoustics, the study and analysis of
how sound interacted with rock art and other ritual practices of ancient
populations. Indeed, a rock art conference nowadays can hardly be held without
at least one paper on Archaeoacoustics being presented. A PBS documentary
series, How We Got To Now, hosted by
Steven Johnson, devoted its sixth episode to Sound. At the beginnings of the
episode Iegor Reznikoff was introduced inside one of the European painted caves
- Arcy Sur Cure, in northern France. Reznikoff, who is a professor at the
Philosophy Department of the University of Paris at Nanterre, eloquently
rhapsodized about the role of sound and echoes in cave art and ritual.
Three rhinoceroses, a site
of naturally high resonance
(Reznikoff 2002:48).
Rouffignac cave, France.
Photo: donsmaps.com.
"In a prehistoric cave, one of
the most impressive experiences is to discover the cave, walking in complete or
almost complete darkness, and all while making sounds (preferably vocal ones)
and to listen to the answer of the cave. In order to figure out where the
sounds come from - from far away or from nearby - and whether there is
somewhere a strong resonance or not: all this in order to ascertain the
direction in which one may proceed further on. Because out vision is limited by
darkness, resonance is the only way to know how long or deep the space ahead
is. This represents one use of the voice and of the hearing as a sonar device,
and there is no doubt that Palaeolithic tribes who visited and decorated the
caves proceeded in this way; indeed, in irregular shaped galleries or tunnels,
neither oil lamps nor even torches light further than a few meters. This sonar
method works: in many cases, proceeding into the direction of the strongest
answer of the cave will lead to the locations of paintings. This way of moving
around in darkness demonstrates the main importance of sound in discovering
space and in proceeding through it; to be sure, it reminds one of the first
perception of space the child has in the world of the mother's womb." (Reznikoff 2005:Section 2.5)
Salmon, an area of increased
resonance. Salle des Vagues,
Arcy-sur-Cure cave, Burgundy,
France. Photo: M. Girard.
This is one
of his conclusions, that sound (echoes) was used in navigating caves. He talks
about the human body as a sonar device, emitting and receiving sound and
analyzing the information contained in the returning signal. He also goes into
sound as an integral part of ritual; music, vocalization, imitating animal
calls, etc. His analysis is not rigorously scientific. The sound production in
his studies is vocal and his instrument for receiving the sound is aural, his
ear. This is, of course, the way it would have been with our ancestors too.
The ‘Diamond incrusted’ mammoth
in the most resonant location of the
main cave (Reznikoff 2002).
Arcy-sur-Cure (Burgundy, France).
Photograph M. Girard).
"The method consists in
studying, or rather listening to, the resonance of sounds in terms of its
intensity and duration at different points along the cave walls. In this way, the
places with the greatest resonance can be located, or, more generally, the
places where the quality of the resonance changes significantly. A map of
resonance in the cave galleries can thus be drawn up. In acoustical terms, the
changes correspond to the alternation along the cave wall of nodes and
antinodes for those frequencies that are characteristic of particular parts of
the cave. The matter can, however, be rather complex, because the pitches that
make different parts of the cave resound must be identified at the same time.
In different parts of the cave, different and, possibly, many pitches (even if
only the basic sound is taken into account) can be peculiar to its resonance
since its form can be very complex; we are thus faced with two variables:
location and pitch." (1995:503)
Ariege, France, here the resonance
lasts for 5 seconds (Reznikoff 2001:49).
Photograph: Internet, public domain.
Reznikoff
comes to three main conclusions - what he calls Principles.
1. The
density of paintings in a location is proportional to the intensity of the
resonance in this location. He qualifies this by admitting that "it is unreasonable to expect all the
locations with good resonance to be painted, there are in general too many and
some are unsuitable or inaccessible." (Also some painted sites have
poor acoustic properties. What does that say about his theory?)
2. Most
ideal resonant locations are adorned with paintings or signs. (I am really
skeptical here, I know of all sorts of good echo sites with no rock art.)
3. Certain
signs are accounted for only in relation to sound. (Possibly true, but since he
does not identify those signs we really cannot judge) (Reznikoff 2008:4140)
So, is
Reznikoff right? Well, I can see some logic in his argument that sound would be
helpful in navigating caves. But would his results be replicable by another
researcher using a different voice and ears, or would personal differences lead
to differing results? In other words can his experimental results be replicated
by another experimenter (the primary test for scientific validity)? I think
not.
I must
admit I enjoy reading his papers, they are not dry scientific discourses. He is
eloquent, even poetic, in describing his beliefs - but is he correct in his
assumptions? I will even agree with him to a certain extent. I do have this
caveat, however. I have written previously about my belief that the question of
acoustics is overdone in current rock art studies. The fact that rock art sites
often produce impressive echoes is assumed to be causative, that the rock art
is there because of the echo. As I have written elsewhere, desirable rock
panels for rock art and for causing echoes are coincident, but we have no proof
that they are related. So, Reznikoff's method of vocalizing and then listing
his impressions of what he hears is certainly not scientific, his results must
be predominately subjective, but that does not invalidate his efforts, and he
surely must be having a lot of fun. Part 2 of this essay will be posted next week.
NOTE:
Some images in this posting were retrieved from the internet with a search for
public domain photographs. If any of these images are not intended to be public
domain, I apologize, and will happily provide the picture credits if the owner
will contact me with them. For further information on these reports you should
read the originals listed below.
REFERENCES:
Reznikoff,
Iegor
1995 On the Sound Dimension of Prehistoric Painted
Caves and Rocks, p. 541-57, from Musical
Signification: Essays in the Semiotic Theory and Analysis of Music, edited
by Eero Tarasti, De Gruyter Mouton, Berlin and New York.
2002 Prehistoric Painting, Sound and Rocks, from Studien zur Musikarchaologie III, The Archaeology of Sound: Origin and Organization, Deutsches Archaologisches Institut, Istanbul, Verlag Marie Leidorf GmbH, Rahden, Westf.
2002 Prehistoric Painting, Sound and Rocks, from Studien zur Musikarchaologie III, The Archaeology of Sound: Origin and Organization, Deutsches Archaologisches Institut, Istanbul, Verlag Marie Leidorf GmbH, Rahden, Westf.
2005 On
Primitive Elements of Musical Meaning,
Journal of Music and Meaning, vol. 3, Fall 2004/Winter2005, Section 2, http://www.musicandmeaning.net/issues/showArticle.php?artID=3.2
2008 Sound
Resonance in Prehistoric Times: A Study of Palaeolithic Painted Caves and
Rocks, p. 4137-4141, www.acoustics08-paris.org
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment