We rock art enthusiasts have long believed that the beginnings of rock art indicated a certain level of intellectual development in our human ancestors. Now, a remarkable article in the April, 2016, Scientific American (Vol. 314, No. 4), by Dietrich Stout titled Cognitive Psychology/Tales of a Stone Age Neuroscientist, suggests that learning to make rock art may have been an important factor in that intellectual development.
Saturday, May 21, 2016
ROCK ART, AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF INTELLIGENCE:
Disclaimer: Except for the direct quotes following, the content of this posting is completely my speculation, and neither Dietrich Stout or Scientific American can be held responsible for any mistakes or errors.
We rock art enthusiasts have long believed that the beginnings of rock art indicated a certain level of intellectual development in our human ancestors. Now, a remarkable article in the April, 2016, Scientific American (Vol. 314, No. 4), by Dietrich Stout titled Cognitive Psychology/Tales of a Stone Age Neuroscientist, suggests that learning to make rock art may have been an important factor in that intellectual development.
We rock art enthusiasts have long believed that the beginnings of rock art indicated a certain level of intellectual development in our human ancestors. Now, a remarkable article in the April, 2016, Scientific American (Vol. 314, No. 4), by Dietrich Stout titled Cognitive Psychology/Tales of a Stone Age Neuroscientist, suggests that learning to make rock art may have been an important factor in that intellectual development.
Stout reported that he and his collaborators learned to knap
stone, to recreate Oldowan type stone tools (2.5 to 1.2 million years BP), and
Achulean type stone tools (1.6 million to 200,000 years BP). This process of
learning to knap stone, and then the production of the tools, proceeded with a
series of brain scans to attempt to identify any neural changes.
"We suspected that learning to
knap would also require some degree of neural rewiring. If so, we wanted to
know which circuits were affected. If our idea was correct we hoped to get a
glimpse of whether toolmaking can actually cause, on a small scale, the same
type of anatomical changes in an individual that occurred over the course of
human evolution.
The answer turned out to be a
resounding yes: practice in knapping enhanced white matter tracts connecting
the same frontal and parietal regions identified in our PET and MRI studies,
including the right inferior frontal gyrus of the prefrontal cortex, a region
critical for cognitive control. The extent of these changes could be predicted
from the actual number of hours each subject spent practicing - the more
someone practiced, the more their white matter changed." (Stout
2015:33-34)
Rhinos, Chauvet Cave.
Wikimedia. Public domain
Lion Man, Hohlenstein-Stadel.
Public domain.
But, what excited me more upon reading this is that the
creation of the two tool types left detectable differences in the brain
changes. This leads me to what I believe to the reasonable conclusion that the
creation of any two types of object would have different effects upon the
development of the brain of the creator. In other words, cave painting and
Paleolithic bone and ivory carving would have enhanced the development of the
brains of their creators, and thus I think I can safely assume that this would also
apply to the act of creating petroglyphs and pictographs. That the creation of this rock art not only signaled a
certain level of cognitive development, it actually contributed to that
development, and the different types of creations made different contributions to that
development.
"The results of
our own imaging studies on stone toolmaking led us recently to propose that
neural circuits, including the inferior frontal gyrus, underwent changes to
adapt to the demands of Paleolithic toolmaking and then were co-opted to
support primitive forms of communication using gestures and, perhaps,
vocalizations. This protolinguistic communication would then have been
subjected to selection, ultimately producing the specific adaptations that
support modern human language." (Stout 2016:35)
Westwater Creek, Bookcliffs,
Grand County, Utah.
Photograph: Peter Faris,
September, 1981.
Sproat Lake, Vancouver Island,
British Columbia, Canada.
Photograph: Peter Faris, 1995.
REFERENCE:
Stout, Dietrich
2016 Cognitive
Psychology: Tales of a Stone Age Neuroscientist, pages 28-35, Scientific
American, Volume 314, Number 4, April, 2016.
Wikipedia
Labels:
evolution,
language,
petroglyph,
pictograph,
rock art,
tool making
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment